In a previous issue of
Behavioral and Brain Sciences (vol 20, issue 1, March 1997), “What memory is
for”, an impressive and provocative article by Arthur Glenberg (available here).
The abstract begins: “Let’s
start from scratch in thinking about what memory is for, and consequently, how
it works. Suppose that memory and conceptualization work in the service of
perception and action. In this case, conceptualization is the encoding of
patterns of possible physical interaction with a three-dimensional world. These
patterns are constrained by the structure of the environment, the structure of our
bodies, and memory. Thus, how we perceive and conceive of the environment is
determined by the types of bodies we have. Such a memory would not have
associations. Instead, how concepts become related (and what it means to be
related) is determined by how separate patterns of actions can be combined given
the constraints of our bodies. I call this combination “mesh.” To avoid
hallucination, conceptualization would normally be driven by the environment,
and patterns of action from memory would play a supporting, but automatic,
role. A significant human skill is learning to suppress the overriding
contribution of the environment to conceptualization, thereby allowing memory
to guide conceptualization. The effort used in suppressing input from the
environment pays off by allowing prediction, recollective memory, and language
comprehension. I review theoretical work in cognitive science and empirical
work in memory and language comprehension that suggest that it may be possible
to investigate connections between topics as disparate as infantile amnesia and
mental-model theory.
No comments:
Post a Comment